Skip to main content

Positive psychological well-being predicts lower severe pain in the general population: a 2-year follow-up study of the SwePain cohort

Abstract

Background

Positive psychology indicators like well-being and life satisfaction may play a pivotal role in pain-related outcomes. In this study, we aimed to examine the prospective associations of positive well-being and life satisfaction with pain severity.

Methods and Subjects

This longitudinal study, with a follow-up of 2 years, included 9361 participants (4266 males, 5095 females; mean age: 52.5 years; SD: 17.5) without and with chronic pain (CP) at baseline. All analyses were stratified by the two sub-cohorts—participants without CP (sub-cohort 1) and participants with CP (sub-cohort 2) at baseline. The predictive associations, assessed using ordinal regression in a Generalized Linear Model, were adjusted for baseline potential confounders and reported as odds ratios (ORs) with corresponding 95% confidence intervals (CIs).

Results

After adjustments, in sub-cohort 1 positive well-being at baseline was associated with lower severe pain at follow-up compared to participants with severe distress (OR: 0.64; 95% CI 0.49–0.84; p < 0.001). In sub-cohort 2, both positive well-being and life satisfaction at baseline were associated with lower severe pain at follow-up compared to participants with severe distress and not satisfied with life (OR: 0.80; 95% CI 0.65–0.98; p = 0.031 and OR: 0.82; 95% CI 0.69–0.96; p = 0.014, respectively).

Conclusions

Positive well-being is predictive of lower pain severity both among participants without and with CP at baseline, whereas life satisfaction was found predictive of lower pain severity only for subjects with CP. Future research should emphasize implementing treatments associated with promoting and maintaining positive well-being and life satisfaction in patients who suffer from chronic pain and in risk populations.

Background

Psychological well-being (hereafter referred to as well-being) can be described as having positive emotions (e.g. contentment and happiness), positive life evaluations (e.g. life satisfaction), and functioning well in daily life [1, 2]. This description falls within the hedonic perspective and essentially differs from two other perspectives of well-being—the eudaimonic, a perspective that focuses on purpose in life/optimism, and the social, a perspective that focuses on good social integration/contribution [1, 2]. In any case, well-being is referred to the presence of positive emotions and evaluations and not just absence of negative ones such as anxiety and depression [3]. Well-being is grounded in positive psychology, a scientific discipline that promotes qualities such as positive feelings, perceptions, and behaviours required for people to flourish [4].

Today, well-being has become a critical issue in positive health outcomes [1, 3]. Well-being is considered to be a key factor connected with better health outcomes such as strong health, resilience, recovery, and longevity in both healthy and diseased populations [1, 3, 5, 6]. Such outcomes are of major importance to professionals, patients, and health systems because the personal and economic burden related to ill-being is massive [1]. Cross-sectional and longitudinal studies have demonstrated positive associations between well-being and immune function and neuroendocrine regulation [7, 8], better health behaviours and sleep [7, 9, 10], lower levels of infection [7, 8], decreased risk of illness [7, 11,12,13], less morbidity and lower mortality [5, 6], lower disability [14, 15], decreased distress [16], and increased longevity [17]. Meta-analyses also support the protective role of well-being with respect to overall as well as cardiovascular mortality [18], survival [3], personal failures across life span [19], cancer outcomes, negative pregnancy outcomes, and physical symptoms [20].

Given this background, well-being is likely to be important to monitor when considering possible interventions in pain populations with high disability and health care costs [21, 22]. However, there is only limited evidence that suggests that well-being may have a crucial protective role in pain-related outcomes. A cross-sectional study suggested that positive well-being is associated with low pain intensity and depression [23]. Another cross-sectional study highlighted the importance of the association between life satisfaction and low pain intensity and depressive symptoms [24]. A feasibility trial investigating a tailored positive psychology intervention also resulted in lower pain intensity and better pain control in patients with chronic pain compared to controls [25]. Two other prospective studies provided evidence for an association between positive well-being and reduced risk of incident arthritis and arthritis disability [13, 14].

Uncertainty also exists with respect to the associations among diseased populations, whereas the associations among healthy populations are quite firm [1]. Additionally, predictive associations of well-being with pain-related outcomes are quite sparse. Therefore, this study focuses on the following two aims: (1) to investigate the baseline (T0) association of positive well-being and life satisfaction with five grades of pain severity at a 2-year follow-up (T1) in a large cohort of the general population without chronic pain at T0 (sub-cohort 1) and with chronic pain at T0 (sub-cohort 2), and (2) to assess these associations with respect to changes in pain severity at the 2-year follow-up. We hypothesized that positive well-being and life satisfaction are associated with decreased risk (i.e. protective factors) of severe pain in both sub-cohorts at T1 after controlling for baseline confounders.

Materials and methods

Participants and procedures

Based on a large Swedish population-based cohort study, [26, 27] this study assesses biopsychosocial aspects of pain in a sampling frame gathered from the Swedish Total Population Register. No longitudinal analyses have been reported for this cohort. Baseline data (T0) were collected using a representative stratified random sample of 34,000 individuals from the general population in south-eastern Sweden. The random sampling was stratified by gender and municipality to reach individuals living in urban and rural areas [26]. Data were collected by Statistics Sweden. The selected individuals received a postal questionnaire in March 2013, which could be returned either by post or electronically. A reminder was sent to non-responders after 2 weeks; if necessary, another reminder was sent 2 weeks later. The collection of questionnaires ended in May 2013. Follow-up data (T1) were collected 2 years later. Only individuals who completed and returned the first questionnaire were eligible to participate in the follow-up assessment. Eligible individuals received a postal survey in March 2015, which could be returned by post or electronically. Two reminders were sent. The collection of the follow-up data ended in May 2015. Both surveys at T0 and T1 included the same questions. The survey for this study is described below. Completion of the postal survey was deemed to be the agreement of participant’s informed consent. The study was approved by the local ethics committee of Linköping University, Sweden (Dnr: 2011 72/31), and this research was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of the World Medical Association.

For the present longitudinal study, inclusion criteria were age between 18 and 85 years and individuals who answered at both T0 and T1 regardless of their pain status (Fig. 1). Chronic pain (CP) was defined by a single question regarding the presence and duration of pain: “Do you usually have pain?” Three response options were available: (1) no; (2) yes, with less duration than 3 months; and (3) yes, with duration of more than 3 months. Individuals who answered 3 were classified as CP, and individuals who answered 1 or 2 were classified as no CP. Hence, this study had two sub-cohorts: sub-cohort 1—individuals without CP at baseline irrespective of their CP status at follow-up; and sub-cohort 2—individuals with CP at baseline irrespective of their CP status at follow-up (Fig. 1). This strategy allowed us to include those individuals who shift from no pain at T0 to CP at T1 and vice versa and to examine whether the associations for these two sub-cohorts differ.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Flow chart outline of the inclusion of participants for this study. CPGs Chronic pain grades, T0 baseline, T1 2-year follow-up

Reporting of the results from this study was done in accordance with the Strengthening the Reporting of Observational Studies in Epidemiology (STROBE) statement (see Additional file 1) [28].

Measurements

Outcome variable

Pain severity

The Swedish version of the chronic pain grade (CPG) scale was chosen to capture pain severity at T0 and T1. CPG was designed to assess global pain severity in three dimensions: intensity, persistence, and disability [29,30,31]. Previous reports indicated that CPG is a valid and reliable instrument [32,33,34,35].

The intensity dimension (denoted CPG intensity) was composed of three items: (1) average pain intensity for the previous 7 days using a numeric rating scale (NRS; 0 = no pain and 10 = worst imaginable pain); (2) pain intensity at its worst the previous 4 weeks using the NRS; and (3) average pain intensity previous 4 weeks using the NRS. CPG intensity was calculated as the average of these three scales and multiplying this by ten, yielding a range of 0 to 100.

The persistence dimension (denoted CPG persistence) indicated the number of days the participants were disabled by pain during the previous 4 weeks (i.e. number of days unable to work, study, or perform household work due to pain). To use the five grades of CPG, the number of days was scaled as follows: 0 point = 0–1 day; 1 point = 2–3 days; 2 points = 4–5 days; and 3 points > 5 days.

The interference dimension (CPG disability) consisted of two items: (1) how often has pain kept you from working, studying, or performing household work the previous 4 weeks and (2) how often pain has kept you from leisure activities, social activities, or family activities the previous 4 weeks. Both these items were assessed using the NRS (0 = no interference and 10 = unable to carry on these activities). CPG disability was calculated as the average of the two items and multiplying this number by ten to yield a range of 0 to 100. For the grades of CPG, the scores were converted to points according to instrument instructions: [29] 0 point = 0–29; 1 point = 30–49; 2 points = 50–69; and 3 points > 70 points. Disability points were calculated as the sum of the points for both CPG persistence and CPG disability, resulting a possible range of 0 to 6.

Next, we categorized the different grades of CPG as previously proposed: [29] (1) CPG-0: no pain and no disability (i.e. individuals without CP); (2) CPG-I: low CGP intensity (< 50) and low disability points (< 3 points); (3) CPG-II: High CGP intensity (> 50) and low disability (< 3 points); (4) CPG-III: High disability (moderately limiting; 3–4 disability points regardless of CPG intensity); and (5) CPG-IV: High disability (severely limiting; 5–6 disability points regardless of CPG intensity. The higher grade, the more pain severity. The grades of CPG were treated as an ordinal outcome to predict the cumulative ordered log-odds of being in a higher pain severity grade with reference to both intensity and disability (CPG-0, CPG-I, CPG-II, CPG-III, and CPG-IV).

Predictor variables

Two indicators of psychological well-being within the hedonic perspective were considered—positive well-being and life satisfaction.

Psychological well-being

To measure well-being, we used the General Well-Being Scale (GWBS) [36]. The GWBS, a common instrument for assessing positive well-being and distress, consists of 18 items that yield a total score ranging from 0 to 110 (high score indicating positive well-being and low score indicating distress). The interval 0–60 reflects severe distress, 61–72 moderate distress, and 73–110 positive well-being. The first 14 questions use a six-point rating scale (ranging from 0 to 5) that represents intensity or frequency, and the remaining four items use an 11-point rating scale with the end-points 0 (very concerned) and 10 (not concerned at all). The instrument has provided good internal consistency, test–retest reliability, and validity. [36] GWBS can also produce six subscales: [37] Anxiety, Depression, Positive well-being, Self-control, Vitality, and General health. The data were categorized as severe distress, moderate distress, and positive well-being according to proposed intervals [36].

Life satisfaction

To capture the individual’s estimations of global satisfaction with life, we used the domain ‘global satisfaction’—one domain of the 11 domains listed in the Life Satisfaction Questionnaire (LISAT-11) [38]. The LISAT-11 measures levels of satisfaction along a six-point rating scale from 1 (very dissatisfied) to 6 (very satisfied). Although the LISAT-11 includes domain-specific satisfaction in ten other domains, these domains were out of the scope of our project, so we did not assess them. The data were dichotomized as ‘satisfied’ (answer options 5 and 6) and ‘not satisfied’ (answer options 1–4), as previously proposed [39]. The instrument has been shown to provide good validity [38, 39].

Potential confounders

We identified nine covariates (other than baseline CPGs) as potential confounders based on acknowledged associations between these factors and both CP and psychological well-being/life satisfaction: age (older adults vs. adults), sex (women vs. men), county of birth (abroad vs. born in Sweden), marital status (married vs. other), education (university vs. other), employment (unemployment vs. employment), household income (≥ median = high income vs. < median = low income), multimorbidity (multimorbidity vs. single morbidity), sleep problems (yes vs. no). Multimorbidity was assessed by a self-reported questionnaire of 12 common diagnoses of various disorders, as described elsewhere [40, 41]. Briefly, these diagnoses included traumatic injuries, rheumatoid arthritis and osteoarthritis, cardiovascular disorders, pulmonary disorders, depressive disorders, anxiety disorders, gastrointestinal disorders, disorders of the central nervous system, urogenital disorders, skin disorders, tumours and cancer, and metabolic disorders. Multimorbidity was defined as the presence of two or more diagnoses of various disorders while single morbidity was defined as the presence of one or none [42]. The assessment of sleep problems was based on a single question: Do you have trouble falling or staying asleep (Yes or No)?

Data analysis

All statistical analyses were performed using IBM SPSS Statistics (version 23.0; IBM Inc., New York, USA). Two-sided statistical tests were used and p < 0.05 was regarded as significant. All analyses were stratified by the two sub-cohorts—participants without CP at T0 (CPG = 0) and participants with CP at T0 (CPG > 0). Distributions and descriptive statistics were examined for all variables at both T0 and T1. Means and standard deviations (SDs) for continuous variables and frequencies with percentages (n; %) for categorical variables were calculated. The associations of the baseline predictor variables (positive well-being and life satisfaction) with the outcome variable (pain severity as derived by the five ordered CPGs) were analysed using ordinal regression under a Generalized Linear Model (GLZ), which allows the outcome variable to have an ordinal distribution [43, 44]. GLZ is a flexible generalization of ordinary linear regression and can be used to analyse data with binary, discrete, or continuous outcomes [44].

We used GLZ with an ordinal distribution and a cumulative logit link function. The statistical significance of the model was examined by the Wald test. [44] Multicollinearity among covariates was estimated through variance inflation factor (VIF) with a cut-off score of > 2 as an indicator of multicollinearity [45]. Because the VIF was < 2, all covariates were included in the analysis. We present three models: one unadjusted in which crude odds ratios (ORs) with corresponding 95% confidence intervals (CIs) were calculated for both baseline predictors studied (i.e. well-being and life satisfaction) in a simultaneous model; one adjusted in which both baseline predictors studied were adjusted for the nine covariates, as previously described, excluding the baseline CPGs; and one fully adjusted model including CPGs at baseline in which both baseline predictors studied were adjusted for the nine covariates and the baseline CPGs as well. However, the fully adjusted model cannot be applied to sub-cohort 1 since everyone had a CPG = 0.

Finally, when significant associations between the baseline covariates and pain severity as measured by CPGs were found, a multiple post hoc sensitivity analysis was additionally performed to show how the interaction between well-being and life satisfaction depended on these selected participant characteristics (age, sex, county of birth, marital status, education, employment, household income, multimorbidity, and sleep problems in the association with pain severity, i.e. weaker vs. stronger).

Results

Study participation

The flowchart of the sample selection is depicted in Fig. 1. At T0, 15,563 individuals (46% men, 54% women) completed and returned the questionnaire, a 46% response. Of these, 11,386 (74%) completed the survey at T1. The dropout analysis showed that the response rate at T0 was lower among men, singles, and participants born abroad, and the response rate at T1 declined for younger ages, men, singles, secondary educated, unemployed, having lower household income, multimorbidity, higher functional impairment, being severely distressed, and born abroad (Table 1). Of the 11,386 individuals who participated at both T0 and T1, 2025 were further excluded because of missing data on the CPG questionnaires at either T0 or T1. Hence, 9361 individuals were included in this longitudinal study. The sub-cohort 1 and the sub-cohort 2 consisted of 5693 and 3668 participants, respectively (Fig. 1).

Table 1 Description of the sociodemographic characteristics and study measures at both baseline (T0) and at the 2-year follow-up (T1), and characteristics of non-participants at T0 and T1

Descriptive characteristics

The total sample consisted of 4266 men (45.6%) and 5095 women (54.4%) and the mean age was 52.5 (SD = 17.5) years. In sub-cohort 1, the mean age was 51.0 (SD = 18.3) years and 49.6% were women. In sub-cohort 2, the mean age was 54.7 years (SD = 8) years and 62.0% were women. In both sub-cohorts, women were younger than men. Table 2 illustrates the baseline characteristics for the two sub-cohorts stratified by the three nominal categories of the well-being (severe distress, moderate distress, and positive well-being) and by the two nominal categories of the life satisfaction (satisfied and not satisfied).

Table 2 Characteristics of the two study sub-cohorts classified according to baseline psychological well-being and life satisfaction

Psychological well-being and life satisfaction in relation to chronic pain grades

Percentages of well-being, life satisfaction, and CPGs were fairly stable over time for the two sub-cohorts (see Additional files 2 and 3). In sub-cohort 1 without CP at baseline, the overall proportions of positive well-being and life satisfaction were highest among CPG-0 at T1 (see Additional file 2). In sub-cohort 2 with CP at baseline, the report of severe distress and not satisfied was more noticeable for those with CPG I and II at both time points (see Additional file 3).

Prospective associations of psychological well-being and life satisfaction in relation to chronic pain grades

The unadjusted and adjusted ordinal regression analyses for both sub-cohorts are presented in Table 3. In sub-cohort 1, the adjusted analysis (i.e. corrected for the nine baseline potential confounders) showed that participants with positive well-being had significantly lower cumulative ordered odds of being in a higher CPG grade. Life satisfaction, conversely, was not statistically associated with CPGs in sub-cohort 1 (Table 3). Furthermore, in the adjusted analysis, we found that being a woman, multimorbidity, and sleep problems were significantly associated with CPGs at T1 (see Additional file 4; Sub-cohort 1). The sensitivity analyses revealed that the association between positive well-being and pain severity is moderated by being a woman (p = 0.029), multimorbidity (p < 0.001), and sleep problems (p = 0.004).

Table 3 Ordinal regression analyses via GLZ models of CPGs at T1 using psychological well-being and life satisfaction at T0 as independent variables for the two study sub-cohorts

In sub-cohort 2, the adjusted analysis (i.e. corrected for the nine baseline potential confounders) showed that participants with positive well-being and moderate distress, with severe distress as a reference, had significantly lower cumulative ordered odds of being in a higher CPG grade. In this model, life satisfaction was non-significant (Table 3). The fully adjusted model including CPGs at baseline showed that participants with both positive well-being and life satisfaction had significantly lower cumulative ordered odds of being in a higher CPG grade (Table 3). Moreover, in the fully adjusted mode, we found that CPGs at T0 were associated with changes in CPGs at T1. The strongest association was found for CPG-IV compared to CPG-I (see Additional file 5).

In the fully adjusted model, we also found that being a woman, born abroad, married, university educated, multimorbidity, and sleep problems were significantly associated with CPGs at T1 (see Additional file 4; Sub-cohort 2). The sensitivity analyses revealed that the association between positive well-being and pain severity is moderated by multimorbidity (p < 0.040) and sleep problems (p = 0.016). The same analysis between life satisfaction and the above-mentioned covariates showed that none emerged as a moderator of our findings (p = 0.059) for the interaction for all covariates.

Discussion

In this longitudinal study, we found that well-being at baseline, especially positive well-being, predicted lower risk of severe pain at the 2-year follow-up both for participants with no CP at baseline (sub-cohort 1) and for participants with CP (sub-cohort 2) after baseline adjustments. Although the groups with high/low well-being were quite different at baseline with respect to other background factors, the predictive associations remained after adjustments. In addition, life satisfaction was protective against increased pain severity during the same period for sub-cohort 2, but not for sub-cohort 1. Pain severity at baseline was a relatively strong significant predictor of pain severity at the 2-year follow-up. However, this adjustment did not alter the protective significant associations with positive well-being, life satisfaction, and pain severity, further enhancing confidence in our findings. Therefore, our study found that positive well-being has a favourable effect on pain severity in both sub-cohorts.

However, our findings are congruent with previous findings, which have shown that well-being is a strong predictor of positive health outcomes [3, 4, 18,19,20]. With respect to pain populations, our results are in agreement with findings from cross-sectional studies [23, 24]. Likewise, two prospective studies have also reported similar results [13, 14]. Briefly, in a 2-year cohort study of 1084 Mexican-Americans with arthritis, participants with high positive well-being reported a significantly lower risk of disability (almost 55%) after controlling for various baseline cofounders [14]. A considerable related result emerged from another longitudinal study including a total of 13,594 participants aged ≥ 50 years and with a follow-up of over 9 years [13]. However, we cannot directly compare our results with other studies because of dissimilar populations, outcomes, and assessments of well-being. The possibility that a reciprocal association between well-being and ill-being may exist should also be considered [1, 46]. In any case, further prospective epidemiological research is required to confirm the temporal links between illness and well-being in pain populations.

Our results suggest a more protective effect of well-being in sub-cohort 1 compared to sub-cohort 2. This effect can be attributed to the fact that diseased populations, especially those in chronicity, have lower cumulative exposure to well-being than healthy populations and greater exposure to ill-being over time. Nevertheless, the difference in ORs was not very large between the two sub-cohorts, indicating an overall favourable effect of well-being in general populations with and without pain. The finding that life satisfaction was associated with less pain severity in sub-cohort 2 but not in sub-cohort 1, although not easy to interpret, may indicate that positive evaluations of life may vary across a lifespan depending on the context they occurred [47]. Thus, there is the possibility that diseased populations have a better hedonic adaptation—i.e. these populations evaluate their personal success and way of living more positively and as such this belief serves as a defence mechanism against illness, whereas in healthy populations life satisfaction may decline over time [48]. These speculations require further examination.

Testing a model containing baseline pain adjustments in sub-cohort 2, we found that participants who reported high severe pain at baseline had more than ten times increased risk of severe pain at the 2-year follow-up. These results provide further evidence that pain severity is the strongest predictor of worse future pain severity. More importantly, these results demonstrate that well-being and pain severity are independently and differentially associated with pain severity risk, at least in pain populations. Additionally, the association between positive well-being and reduced risk of pain severity seems to be partly conditioned by other baseline factors in both sub-cohorts. In sub-cohort 1, we found that the association was weaker in women compared to men, in multimorbidity compared to single morbidity, and in sleep problems compared to no sleep problems. In sub-cohort 2, we found that the association was weaker only in multimorbidity compared to single morbidity and in sleep problems compared to no sleep problems. Our results are consistent with previous studies that displayed a significant association between well-being, sex, and morbidities with higher well-being in men [49] and lower in patients with multimorbidities [46]. In contrast, none of the baseline covariates emerged as a moderator between the association of life satisfaction and pain severity.

Strengths and limitations

This is the first study to reveal a predictive value of positive well-being and life satisfaction in a large cohort of the general population with and without chronic pain, alongside with its prospective nature, detailed exposure data, and confounder information. Furthermore, two internationally accepted and commonly used measures of well-being [38, 39] within the hedonic perspective were chosen to address our study hypothesis [1, 2]. However, our study has some shortcomings. One limitation is that all the assessments reported here are based on self-report measurements, so it is possible that our results are overestimated. It has been reported that self-reported health measures are more likely to exhibit biased associations because they exhibit stronger associations between well-being and health outcomes than health assessments made by physicians [50]. Although we had sufficient sample sizes per nominal category of well-being and life satisfaction, the proportion of participants reported higher well-being and life satisfaction was substantially larger. Thus, a ceiling effect might be presented [51]. Furthermore, we observed that the response rate at follow-up weakened among individuals with stronger indicators of biopsychological stress, indicating a response bias or a selection bias. Finally, we did not adjust for baseline-specific chronic pain conditions (i.e. traumatic or non-traumatic neck pain, low back pain, fibromyalgia). These adjustments should be considered in future studies.

Acknowledging these caveats, our study highlights the importance of well-being not only as an additional important factor worthy to be assessed in clinical daily practice, but also as a possible new treatment option in pain populations. Although the pathways connecting well-being to positive outcomes remain unknown, there is evidence that well-being may receive its protective effect through three mechanisms [7, 16, 52,53,54]. It has been proposed that it can effectively reinforce one’s physiological and biological functioning, and the effective functioning, in turn, retains the health state status in healthy populations or may benefit recovery in diseased populations [7, 16, 52,53,54]. It can further promote individuals to embrace healthy habits and practices such as cessation of smoking, physical exercise, weight control, and healthy sleep habits [7, 9, 10]. Lastly, some theoretical models proposed that well-being can buffer the negative effects of stress by helping individuals embrace adaptive coping skills for stressful life events [19, 55]. However, it is not known if these three mechanisms function independently, are inter-correlated, or are simply reversed associations [1]. For this reason, future experimental studies should examine more thoroughly these pathways of positive well-being in pain populations.

Conclusions

This study found that hedonic measures of well-being are protective factors against pain severity. Well-being, therefore, should be considered when designing or modifying pain treatment interventions. This study also suggests that health care providers should examine ways of improving and preserving positive well-being in populations with and without pain. The potential pathways linking well-being to positive pain-related outcomes must also be thoroughly examined in future studies, since pain appears to have a complex biological, psychological, and social function. Future studies may want to assess overall well-being using the three perspectives (i.e. hedonic, eudaimonic, and social) to identify the independent associations related to positive pain-related outcomes.

Availability of data and materials

Due to the Swedish law regarding the type of data including grounds of confidentiality and anonymity, data will not be available.

Abbreviations

CI:

confidence interval

CP:

chronic pain

CPG:

chronic pain grade

GLZ:

Generalized Linear Model

GWBS:

the General Well-Being Scale

LISAT:

Life Satisfaction Questionnaire

NRS:

numeric rating scale

OR:

odds ratio

SD:

standard deviation

STROBE:

Strengthening the Reporting of Observational Studies in Epidemiology

VIF:

variance inflation factor

References

  1. Steptoe A, Deaton A, Stone AA. Subjective wellbeing, health, and ageing. Lancet. 2015;385(9968):640–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Gallagher MW, Lopez SJ, Preacher KJ. The hierarchical structure of well-being. J Pers. 2009;77(4):1025–50.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Lamers SM, Bolier L, Westerhof GJ, Smit F, Bohlmeijer ET. The impact of emotional well-being on long-term recovery and survival in physical illness: a meta-analysis. J Behav Med. 2012;35(5):538–47.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Seligman ME, Csikszentmihalyi M. Positive psychology. An introduction. Am Psychol. 2000;55(1):5–14.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Birket-Smith M, Hansen BH, Hanash JA, Hansen JF, Rasmussen A. Mental disorders and general well-being in cardiology outpatients—6-year survival. J Psychosom Res. 2009;67(1):5–10.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Moskowitz JT, Epel ES, Acree M. Positive affect uniquely predicts lower risk of mortality in people with diabetes. Health Psychol. 2008;27(1S):S73–82.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Ryff CD, Singer BH, Dienberg Love G. Positive health: connecting well-being with biology. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2004;359(1449):1383–94.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  8. Steptoe A, Wardle J, Marmot M. Positive affect and health-related neuroendocrine, cardiovascular, and inflammatory processes. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2005;102(18):6508–12.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Folkman S, Moskowitz JT. Positive affect and the other side of coping. Am Psychol. 2000;55(6):647–54.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Grant N, Wardle J, Steptoe A. The relationship between life satisfaction and health behavior: a cross-cultural analysis of young adults. Int J Behav Med. 2009;16(3):259–68.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Ostir GV, Markides KS, Peek MK, Goodwin JS. The association between emotional well-being and the incidence of stroke in older adults. Psychosom Med. 2001;63(2):210–5.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Davidson KW, Mostofsky E, Whang W. Don’t worry, be happy: positive affect and reduced 10-year incident coronary heart disease: the Canadian Nova Scotia Health Survey. Eur Heart J. 2010;31(9):1065–70.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  13. Okely JA, Cooper C, Gale CR. Wellbeing and arthritis incidence: the survey of health, ageing and retirement in Europe. Ann Behav Med. 2016;50(3):419–26.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  14. Fisher MN, Snih SA, Ostir GV, Goodwin JS. Positive affect and disability among older Mexican Americans with arthritis. Arthritis Rheum. 2004;51(1):34–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Brummett BH, Morey MC, Boyle SH, Mark DB. Prospective study of associations among positive emotion and functional status in older patients with coronary artery disease. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2009;64(4):461–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Ong AD. Pathways Linking Positive Emotion and Health in Later Life. Curr Direct Psychol Sci. 2010;19(6):358–62.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Steptoe A, Wardle J. Enjoying life and living longer. Arch Intern Med. 2012;172(3):273–5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Chida Y, Steptoe A. Positive psychological well-being and mortality: a quantitative review of prospective observational studies. Psychosom Med. 2008;70(7):741–56.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Lyubomirsky S, King L, Diener E. The benefits of frequent positive affect: does happiness lead to success? Psychol Bull. 2005;131(6):803–55.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Rasmussen HN, Scheier MF, Greenhouse JB. Optimism and physical health: a meta-analytic review. Ann Behav Med. 2009;37(3):239–56.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  21. Vos T, Flaxman AD, Naghavi M, Lozano R, Michaud C, Ezzati M, Shibuya K, Salomon JA, Abdalla S, Aboyans V, et al. Years lived with disability (YLDs) for 1160 sequelae of 289 diseases and injuries 1990–2010: a systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2010. Lancet. 2012;380(9859):2163–96.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  22. Bernfort L, Gerdle B, Rahmqvist M, Husberg M, Levin LA. Severity of chronic pain in an elderly population in Sweden–impact on costs and quality of life. Pain. 2015;156(3):521–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Furrer A, Michel G, Terrill AL, Jensen MP, Muller R. Modeling subjective well-being in individuals with chronic pain and a physical disability: the role of pain control and pain catastrophizing. Disabil Rehabil. 2019;41(5):498–507.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Stalnacke BM. Life satisfaction in patients with chronic pain—relation to pain intensity, disability, and psychological factors. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat. 2011;7:683–9.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  25. Muller R, Gertz KJ, Molton IR, Terrill AL, Bombardier CH, Ehde DM, Jensen MP. Effects of a Tailored positive psychology intervention on well-being and pain in individuals with chronic pain and a physical disability: a feasibility trial. Clin J Pain. 2016;32(1):32–44.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Grimby-Ekman A, Gerdle B, Bjork J, Larsson B. Comorbidities, intensity, frequency and duration of pain, daily functioning and health care seeking in local, regional, and widespread pain—a descriptive population-based survey (SwePain). BMC Musculoskelet Disord. 2015;16:165.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  27. Larsson B, Gerdle B, Bjork J, Grimby-Ekman A. Pain sensitivity and its relation to spreading on the body, intensity, frequency, and duration of pain: a cross-sectional population-based study (SwePain). Clin J Pain. 2017;33(7):579–87.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. von Elm E, Altman DG, Egger M, Pocock SJ, Gotzsche PC, Vandenbroucke JP, Initiative S. The Strengthening the Reporting of Observational Studies in Epidemiology (STROBE) statement: guidelines for reporting observational studies. Prev Med. 2007;45(4):247–51.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Von Korff M, Ormel J, Keefe FJ, Dworkin SF. Grading the severity of chronic pain. Pain. 1992;50(2):133–49.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Von Korff M, Dworkin SF, Le Resche L. Graded chronic pain status: an epidemiologic evaluation. Pain. 1990;40(3):279–91.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Von Korff M. Studying the natural history of back pain. Spine (Phila Pa 1976). 1994;19(18 Suppl):2041S–6S.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Smith BH, Penny KI, Purves AM, Munro C, Wilson B, Grimshaw J, Chambers WA, Smith WC. The Chronic Pain Grade questionnaire: validation and reliability in postal research. Pain. 1997;71(2):141–7.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Underwood MR, Barnett AG, Vickers MR. Evaluation of two time-specific back pain outcome measures. Spine (Phila Pa 1976). 1999;24(11):1104–12.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  34. Elliott AM, Smith BH, Smith WC, Chambers WA. Changes in chronic pain severity over time: the chronic pain grade as a valid measure. Pain. 2000;88(3):303–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Vingard E, Mortimer M, Wiktorin C, Pernold RPTG, Fredriksson K, Nemeth G, Alfredsson L, Musculoskeletal Intervention Center-Norrtalje Study G. Seeking care for low back pain in the general population: a two-year follow-up study: results from the MUSIC-Norrtalje Study. Spine (Phila Pa 1976). 2002;27(19):2159–65.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Fazio AF. A concurrent validational study of the NCHS General Well-Being Schedule. Vital Health Stat 2. 1977;73:1–53.

    Google Scholar 

  37. McDowell I. Measuring health: a guide to rating scales and questionnaires. 3rd ed./McDowell I. editor. New York, Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2006.

  38. Fugel-Meyer AR, Fugl-Meyer KS. The coping process after traumatic brain injury. Scand J Rehabil Med Suppl. 1988;17:51–3.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Fugl-Meyer AR, Melin R, Fugl-Meyer KS. Life satisfaction in 18- to 64-year-old Swedes: in relation to gender, age, partner and immigrant status. J Rehabil Med. 2002;34(5):239–46.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Dragioti E, Larsson B, Bernfort L, Levin LA, Gerdle B. Prevalence of different pain categories based on pain spreading on the bodies of older adults in Sweden: a descriptive-level and multilevel association with demographics, comorbidities, medications, and certain lifestyle factors (PainS65+). J Pain Res. 2016;9:1131–41.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  41. Dragioti E, Bernfort L, Larsson B, Gerdle B, Levin LA. Association of insomnia severity with well-being, quality of life and health care costs: a cross-sectional study in older adults with chronic pain (PainS65+). Eur J Pain. 2018;22(2):414–25.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Wallace E, Salisbury C, Guthrie B, Lewis C, Fahey T, Smith SM. Managing patients with multimorbidity in primary care. BMJ. 2015;350:h176.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Nelder JA, Wedderburn RWM. Generalized Linear Models. J R Stat Soc Ser A (Gen). 1972;135(3):370.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. McCullagh P, Nelder JA. Generalized Linear Models. 2nd ed. UK: Chapman and Hall; 1989.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  45. O’brien R. A caution regarding rules of thumb for variance inflation factors. Qual Quant Int J Methodol. 2007;41(5):673–90.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  46. Wikman A, Wardle J, Steptoe A. Quality of life and affective well-being in middle-aged and older people with chronic medical illnesses: a cross-sectional population based study. PLoS ONE. 2011;6(4):e18952.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  47. Boehm JK, Winning A, Segerstrom S, Kubzansky LD. Variability modifies life satisfaction’s association with mortality risk in older adults. Psychol Sci. 2015;26(7):1063–70.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  48. Sheldon KM, Lyubomirsky S. The challenge of staying happier: testing the Hedonic Adaptation Prevention model. Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 2012;38(5):670–80.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Nilsson KW, Leppert J, Simonsson B, Starrin B. Sense of coherence and psychological well-being: improvement with age. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2010;64(4):347–52.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. George LK, Landerman R. Health and subjective well-being: a replicated secondary data analysis. Int J Aging Hum Dev. 1984;19(2):133–56.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Wang L, Zhang Z, McArdle JJ, Salthouse TA. Investigating ceiling effects in longitudinal data analysis. Multivar Behav Res. 2009;43(3):476–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  52. Pressman SD, Cohen S. Does positive affect influence health? Psychol Bull. 2005;131(6):925–71.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Sin NL. The protective role of positive well-being in cardiovascular disease: review of current evidence, mechanisms, and clinical implications. Curr Cardiol Rep. 2016;18(11):106.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  54. Cohen S, Alper CM, Doyle WJ, Treanor JJ, Turner RB. Positive emotional style predicts resistance to illness after experimental exposure to rhinovirus or influenza a virus. Psychosom Med. 2006;68(6):809–15.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Bower JE, Moskowitz JT, Epel E. Is benefit finding good for your health? Curr Direct Psychol Sci. 2009;18(6):337–41.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Not applicable.

Funding

The Swedish Pain Foundation, Linköping University, and The Medical Research Council of Southeast Sweden contributed financially to this study. The funding bodies did not take part in the collection, analysis, and interpretation of data, in the writing of the manuscript, or in the decision to submit the manuscript for publication.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

BL, JB, ED, and BG were involved in study conception and study design. ED and JB performed the data analyses. ED, JB, and BL drafted the manuscript. All authors discussed the results, and commented on the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Elena Dragioti.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The study was approved by the local ethics committee of Linköping University, Sweden (Dnr: 2011 72/31), and completion of the postal survey was deemed to be the agreement of participants' informed consent.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Additional files

Additional file 1.

STROBE Statement.

Additional file 2: Figure S1.

The relation between psychological well-being, life satisfaction at T0, and pain severity according to CPGs at T1 for sub-cohort 1: Participants without chronic pain at T0 (CPG = 0) Notes: CPGs = Chronic pain grades, T0 = baseline, T1 = 2-year follow-up.

Additional file 3: Figure S2.

The relation between psychological well-being, life satisfaction at T0, and pain severity classified according to CPGs at both T0 and T1 for sub-cohort 2: Participants with chronic pain at T0 (CPGs > 0). Notes: CPGs = Chronic pain grades, T0 = baseline, T1 = 2-year follow-up.

Additional file 4: Figure S3.

Forest plot (OR and 95% CI) summarizing the results of the ordinal regression analysis via GLZ models for the nine baseline covariates for the association between these covariates at T0 and CPGs at T1 for the sub-cohort 1: Participants without chronic pain at T0 (CPG = 0) (left; adjusted model) and sub-cohort 2: Participants with chronic pain at T0 (CPGs > 0) (right; fully adjusted model). An OR > 1 increases the odds of pain severity; an OR < 1 decreases the odds of pain severity. Notes: OR = Odds ratio, CI = Confidence interval, GLZ = Generalized Linear Models for ordinal outcomes. CPGs = Chronic pain grades, T0 = baseline, T1 = 2-year follow-up.

Additional file 5: Figure S4.

Forest plot (OR and 95% CI) summarizing the results of the ordinal regression analysis via GLZ models for the changes of CPGs from T0 to T1 for only the sub-cohort 2: Participants with chronic pain at T0 (CPGs > 0; fully adjusted model). An OR > 1 increases the odds of pain severity; an OR < 1 decreases the odds of pain severity. Notes: OR = Odds ratio, CI = Confidence interval, GLZ = Generalized Linear Models for ordinal outcomes. CPG = Chronic pain grades, T0 = baseline, T1 = 2-year follow-up.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Larsson, B., Dragioti, E., Gerdle, B. et al. Positive psychological well-being predicts lower severe pain in the general population: a 2-year follow-up study of the SwePain cohort. Ann Gen Psychiatry 18, 8 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12991-019-0231-9

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12991-019-0231-9

Keywords